From its beginning, psychiatry did not
restrict itself to “insanity” but strived to explain human behaviour more
generally, extrapolating knowledge from the “mad” persons confined to asylums
into everyday life phenomena, such as anxiety or shyness, sexuality, and
schooling, norm transgressions in general, and criminal law in particular.
Thus, psychiatry was a central player in the expansion of the “triumphalistic”
medical paradigm (Le Fanu, 1999), which saw modern medicine as the royal road
to understanding and alleviation of man’s ailments and sufferings.
The area of crime and punishment has always
attracted human curiosity and imagination. Psychiatrists, being no exception,
have contributed their expertise, often with a humanistic stance against harsh
punishments and penal law retributivism. The psychiatric approach was long
opposed by hard-line moralists and conservatives. Eventually, as the task of
exerting societal control over undesired behaviours to an increasing extent was
assigned to psychiatry, confrontations flared up on a new frontier, namely with
radicals opposed to control structures.
Psychiatry’s perspective on mental disorder
as the cause of crime has thus been one of a “scientific” approach to crime and
punishment as opposed to the legalistic or retributionistic models that came to
be characterized as “moralistic” or even “transcendental”. However, as
experimental settings for testing a causal connection between mental disorder
and crime are virtually impossible to design, there has never been much of an
empirical basis to back the psychiatric stance.
A long time was to elapse
before the question of causation was actually examined beyond the mere
identification of mental problem constellations among subjects who had
committed criminal acts. Today, the notion of a causative role for mental
disorders behind crimes rests mainly on probabilistic inferences from
epidemiological studies (identifying male gender, substance abuse, early-onset disruptive behaviour disorders, especially conduct disorder, and, behind these problems, ADHD, personality disorders and genetic effects as main risk factors). But we have previously discussed the problems in interpreting risk as causation here (for genetic risk, but applicable to all the different types of risk factors listed). More on the results of such studies in the Intro to this thesis, on the theoretical models for such studies here, more on the history of forensic psychiatry here.
Doctors are reluctant to scale down their claims of expertise. Forensic psychiatry is thus a history of overstated expertise and claims to have scientific proofs for very diverse opinions about criminality. Nevertheless, today we know that mental health care needs among offenders are very high and, tragically, often unmet. This is the true challenge for forensic psychiatry.
(This blogpost is partly excerpted from this paper. If you have troubles accessing full-text versions of references, don't hesitate to contact directly for a copy, henrik.anckarsater@neuro.gu.se or in the comment section of this blog).
Doctors are reluctant to scale down their claims of expertise. Forensic psychiatry is thus a history of overstated expertise and claims to have scientific proofs for very diverse opinions about criminality. Nevertheless, today we know that mental health care needs among offenders are very high and, tragically, often unmet. This is the true challenge for forensic psychiatry.
(This blogpost is partly excerpted from this paper. If you have troubles accessing full-text versions of references, don't hesitate to contact directly for a copy, henrik.anckarsater@neuro.gu.se or in the comment section of this blog).
No comments:
Post a Comment